Selected Publications
Electronic versions are provided to ensure timely dissemination of academic work. They can be downloaded for noncommercial purposes. Copyright resides with the respective copyright holders as stated in each article. The files may not be re-posted without permission from copyright holders.
For a dynamically updated list of Rebecca's publications, please check here.
In Press
Planning with Theory of Mind.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[PDF] (1.76 MB)
(In Press).

Perceiving and pursuing legitimate power.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[PDF] (152.76 KB)
(In Press).

2022
Selective responses to faces, scenes, and bodies in the ventral visual pathway of infants.
Current Biology. 32,
[PDF] (3.57 MB)
(2022).

Early concepts of intimacy: Young humans use saliva sharing to infer close relationships.
Science. 375(6578), 311-315.
[PDF] (1.27 MB)
(2022).

Infants infer potential social partners by observing the interactions of their parent with unknown others.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 119(32), e2121390119.
[PDF] (1.43 MB)
(2022).

Using child-friendly movie stimuli to study the development of face, place, and object regions from age 3 to 12 years.
Human Brain Mapping. 43, 2782-2800.
[PDF] (6.63 MB)
(2022).

Advantages and limitations of representing groups in terms of recursive utilities.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 45(E97), 44-45.
[PDF] (2.3 MB)
(2022).

2021
Linking Models of Theory of Mind and Measures of Human Brain Activity.
The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. 209-235.
(2021).
Why has the COVID-19 pandemic increased support for Universal Basic Income?.
Humanities and Social Science Communications. 8(1), 1-12.
[PDF] (1.26 MB)
(2021).

The neuroscience of unmet social needs.
Social Neuroscience. 16(3), 221-231.
[PDF] (1.14 MB)
(2021).

Are some cultures more mind-minded in their moral judgements than others?.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 376(1838),
[PDF] (240.65 KB)
(2021).

A size-adaptive 32-channel array coil for awake infant neuroimaging at 3 Tesla MRI.
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 86, 1773-1785.
[PDF] (1.58 MB)
(2021).

Single-neuronal predictions of others’ beliefs in humans.
Nature. 591(7851), 610-614.
[PDF] (8.4 MB)
(2021).

Leveraging Facial Expressions and Contextual Information to Investigate Opaque Representations of Emotions.
Emotion. 21(1), 96-107.
[PDF] (922.97 KB)
(2021).

2020
Early Signatures and Developmental Change in Brain Regions for Theory of Mind.
Neural Circuit and Cognitive Development. 2, 467-484.
[PDF] (471.74 KB)
(2020).

Preferences for redistribution are sensitive to perceived luck, social homogeneity, war and scarcity.
Cognition. 198, 104234.
[PDF] (2.06 MB)
(2020).

Political preferences and threat perception: opportunities for neuroimaging and developmental research.
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 34, 58-63.
[PDF] (302.77 KB)
(2020).

Reduced Neural Selectivity for Mental States in Deaf Children with Delayed Exposure to Sign Language.
Nature Communications. 11, 3246.
[PDF] (1.3 MB)
(2020).

Response Patterns in the Developing Social Brain are Organized by Social and Emotion Features and Disrupted in Children Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder.
Cortex. 125, 12-29.
[PDF] (1.69 MB)OSFStimuli in OSF page, above.
Stimulus presentation code available here.
(2020).

Processing communicative facial and vocal cues in the superior temporal sulcus.
NeuroImage. 221(1), 117191.
[PDF] (3.52 MB)
(2020).

Acute stress alters neural patterns of value representation for others.
NeuroImage. 209(1), 116497.
[PDF] (1.25 MB)
(2020).

Learning in infancy is active, endogenously motivated, and depends on the prefrontal cortices.
Annual Review of Developmental Psychology. 2, 247-268.
annurev-devpsych-121318-084841.pdf (1.09 MB)
(2020).

Acute social isolation evokes midbrain craving responses similar to hunger.
Nature Neuroscience. 23, 1597-1605.
[PDF] (5.47 MB)
(2020).

2019
Parts-based representations of perceived face movements in the superior temporal sulcus.
Human Brain Mapping.
[PDF] (4 MB)
(2019).

There’s more to “sparkle” than meets the eye: Knowledge of vision and light verbs among congenitally blind and sighted individuals.
Cognition. 189, 105 - 115.
[PDF] (1.75 MB)
(2019).

Development of Predictive Responses in Theory of Mind Brain Regions.
Developmental Science. e12863.
[PDF] (524.66 KB)
(2019).

Leveraging facial expressions and contextual information to investigate opaque representations of emotions.
[PDF] (1.45 MB)
[Supplement] (91.92 KB)
(2019).


The neuroscience of unmet social needs.
Social Neuroscience. 1 - 11.
[PDF] (1.02 MB)
(2019).

2018
Denying humanity: The distinct neural correlates of blatant dehumanization.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 147(7), 1078 - 1093.
[PDF] (942.06 KB)
(2018).

Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years.
Nature Communications.
[PDF] (1.72 MB)OSFOpenNeuro
(2018).

Social Origins of Cortical Face Areas.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[PDF] (1.97 MB)
(2018).

Seeing Other Minds in 3D.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 22(3), 193 - 195.
[PDF] (363.66 KB)
(2018).

Cortical responses to dynamic emotional facial expressions generalize across stimuli, and are sensitive to task-relevance, in adults with and without Autism.
Cortex. 103(2018), 24 - 43.
[PDF] (2.11 MB)
(2018).

“Affective Theory of Mind” and the Function of the Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex.
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 31(1), 36-37.
[PDF] (104.99 KB)
(2018).

The Other and Me: Effects of oxytocin on self-other distinction.
International Journal of Psychophysiology.
[PDF] (2.24 MB)
(2018).

Development of brain networks for social functions: Confirmatory analyses in a large open source dataset.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
[PDF] (2.32 MB)
(2018).

2017
Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm.
Social Psychological and Personality Science. 934 - 942.
[PDF] (256.36 KB)
(2017).

Using individual functional channels of interest to study cortical development with fNIRS.
Developmental Science. e12595.
[PDF] (845.06 KB)
(2017).

Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants.
Nature Communications. 8, 13995.
[PDF] (1.65 MB)
(2017).

Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands.
Cognitive Development. 46, 40 - 50.
[PDF] (464.93 KB)
(2017).

Mentalizing regions represent distributed, continuous, and abstract dimensions of others' beliefs.
NeuroImage. 161, 9 - 18.
[PDF] (1.25 MB)Stimuli
(2017).

High-resolution magnetic resonance imaging reveals nuclei of the human amygdala: manual segmentation to automatic atlas.
NeuroImage. 155(2017), 370 - 382.
[PDF] (1.18 MB)
(2017).

Anterior temporal lobe and the representation of knowledge about people.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 114(16), 4042 - 4044.
[PDF] (592.39 KB)
(2017).

Learning a commonsense moral theory.
Cognition. 167, 107 - 123.
[PDF] (1.68 MB)
(2017).

Formalizing emotion concepts within a Bayesian model of theory of mind.
Current Opinion in Psychology. 17, 15 - 21.
[PDF] (616.93 KB)
(2017).

Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizing.
Nature Human Behaviour.
[PDF] (713.01 KB)
(2017).

Directed network discovery with dynamic network modelling.
Neuropsychologia. 99(2017), 1 - 11.
[PDF] (786.75 KB)
(2017).

Multivariate pattern dependence.
PLOS Computational Biology. 13(11), e1005799.
[PDF] (8.18 MB)
(2017).

2016
Using MRI to Study Developmental Change in Theory of Mind.
Social Cognition: Development Across the Life Span. 210.
(2016).
Decoding task and stimulus representations in face-responsive cortex.
Cognitive Neuropsychology. 33(7-8), 362 - 377.
[PDF] (1.18 MB)
(2016).

Measuring and Modeling Transformations of Information Between Brain Regions with fMRI.
bioRxiv.
[PDF] (1.64 MB)
(2016).

When minds matter for moral judgment: intent information is neurally encoded for harmful but not impure acts.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 1120(3), 476 - 484.
[PDF] (809.4 KB)
(2016).

Moral status of accidents.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 113(17), 4555 - 4557.
[PDF] (529.82 KB)
(2016).

2015
“Visual” Cortex Responds to Spoken Language in Blind Children.
The Journal of Neuroscience. 35(33), 11674 - 11681.
[PDF] (1.18 MB)
(2015).

Neural Representations of Emotion Are Organized around Abstract Event Features.
Current Biology. 25(15), 1945 - 1954.
[PDF] (2.06 MB)
[Raw Feature Ratings] (1.06 MB)Stimuli & Ratings
(2015).


Localizing Pain Matrix and Theory of Mind networks with both verbal and non-verbal stimuli.
NeuroImage. 126(2016), 39 - 48.
[PDF] (1.73 MB)
(2015).

Functional Organization of Social Perception and Cognition in the Superior Temporal Sulcus.
Cerebral Cortex. 25(11), 4596 - 4609.
[PDF] (931.35 KB)Stimuli
(2015).

Empathic control through coordinated interaction of amygdala, theory of mind and extended pain matrix brain regions.
NeuroImage. 114, 105 - 119.
[PDF] (1.78 MB)
(2015).

Amygdala lesions do not compromise the cortical network for false-belief reasoning.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 112153(15), 4827 - 4832.
[PDF] (878.1 KB)
(2015).

Occipital Cortex of Blind Individuals Is Functionally Coupled with Executive Control Areas of Frontal Cortex.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 27(8), 1633 - 1647.
[PDF] (1.08 MB)
(2015).

Structural Connectivity Fingerprints Predict Cortical Selectivity for Multiple Visual Categories across Cortex.
Cerebral Cortex. 26(4), 1668 - 1683.
[PDF] (1.55 MB)
(2015).

A Second Look at Automatic Theory of Mind.
Psychological Science. 26(9), 1353 - 1367.
[PDF] (752.09 KB)
(2015).

2014
Differences in the right inferior longitudinal fasciculus but no general disruption of white matter tracts in children with autism spectrum disorder.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111(5), 1981 - 1986.
[PDF] (682.89 KB)
(2014).

A Common Neural Code for Perceived and Inferred Emotion.
Journal of Neuroscience. 34(48), 15997 - 16008.
[PDF] (1.91 MB)
(2014).

The Neural Bases of Directed and Spontaneous Mental State Attributions to Group Agents.
(Siegel, A., Ed.).PLoS ONE. 9(8), e105341.
[PDF] (399.23 KB)
(2014).

Their pain gives us pleasure: How intergroup dynamics shape empathic failures and counter-empathic responses.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 55(2014), 110 - 125.
[PDF] (693.18 KB)
(2014).

Thinking about seeing: Perceptual sources of knowledge are encoded in the theory of mind brain regions of sighted and blind adults.
Cognition. 133(1), 65 - 78.
[PDF] (1.19 MB)
(2014).

Reduced self-referential neural response during intergroup competition predicts competitor harm.
NeuroImage. 96(2014), 36 - 43.
[PDF] (552.85 KB)
(2014).

2013
Neural Circuit Development and Function in the Brain.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience of Theory of Mind. 3, 367 - 377.
[PDF] (2.26 MB)
(2013).

Functional neuroimaging of theory of mind.
Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Social Neuroscience.
[PDF] (1.6 MB)
(2013).

The new puzzle of Theory of Mind development.
Navigating the Social World: What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us.
[PDF] (272.64 KB)
(2013).

Similar Brain Activation during False Belief Tasks in a Large Sample of Adults with and without Autism.
(Gilbert, S., Ed.).PLoS ONE. 8(9), e75468.
[PDF] (1.98 MB)
(2013).

Theory of Mind: A Neural Prediction Problem.
Neuron. 79(5), 836 - 848.
[PDF] (718.28 KB)
(2013).

People can understand descriptions of motion without activating visual motion brain regions.
Frontiers in Psychology. 4, 537.
[PDF] (3.05 MB)
(2013).

How We Know It Hurts: Item Analysis of Written Narratives Reveals Distinct Neural Responses to Others' Physical Pain and Emotional Suffering.
(Watanabe, K., Ed.).PLoS ONE. 8(4), e63085.
[PDF] (3.16 MB)
(2013).

A Noisy-Channel Account of Crosslinguistic Word-Order Variation.
Psychological Science. 24(7), 1079 - 1088.
[PDF] (1.24 MB)
(2013).

Decoding moral judgments from neural representations of intentions.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 5648 - 5653.
[PDF] (2.45 MB)
(2013).

2012
Matched False-Belief Performance During Verbal and Nonverbal Interference.
Cognitive Science. 36(2012), 1148 - 1156.
[PDF] (130.62 KB)
(2012).

Insights into the origins of knowledge from the cognitive neuroscience of blindness.
Cognitive Neuropsychology. 29(1-2), 56 - 84.
[PDF] (402.93 KB)
(2012).

Teaching Replication.
Perspectives on Psychological Science. 7(6), 600 - 604.
[PDF] (614.43 KB)
(2012).

Typical Neural Representations of Action Verbs Develop without Vision.
Cerebral Cortex. 22(2), 286 - 293.
[PDF] (415.41 KB)
(2012).

Verbal interference suppresses exact numerical representation.
Cognitive Psychology. 64, 74 - 92.
[PDF] (640.51 KB)
(2012).

Thin-slice perception develops slowly.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 112(2), 257 - 264.
[PDF] (635.35 KB)
(2012).

Atypical brain activation patterns during a face-to-face joint attention game in adults with autism spectrum disorder.
Human Brain Mapping. 34(10), 2511 - 2523.
[PDF] (1.16 MB)
(2012).

Look at this: the neural correlates of initiating and responding to bids for joint attention.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 6, 169.
[PDF] (1.26 MB)
(2012).

Theory of Mind Performance in Children Correlates With Functional Specialization of a Brain Region for Thinking About Thoughts.
Child Development. 1853 - 1868.
[PDF] (562.08 KB)OSF
(2012).

The power of being heard: The benefits of ‘perspective-giving’ in the context of intergroup conflict.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 48(4), 855 - 866.
[PDF] (677.34 KB)OSF
(2012).

Distinct roles of the ‘Shared Pain’ and ‘Theory of Mind’ networks in processing others’ emotional suffering.
Neuropsychologia. 50(2), 219 - 231.
[PDF] (1.71 MB)Stimuli
(2012).

Verbal interference suppresses exact numerical representation.
Cognitive Psychology. 64, 74 - 92.
[PDF] (450.67 KB)
(2012).

Social cognition in members of conflict groups: behavioural and neural responses in Arabs, Israelis and South Americans to each other's misfortunes.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 367, 717 - 730.
[PDF] (1.17 MB)
(2012).

2011
Differential selectivity for dynamic versus static information in face-selective cortical regions.
NeuroImage. 56(4), 2356 - 2363.
[PDF] (640.87 KB)
(2011).

A sensitive period for language in the visual cortex: Distinct patterns of plasticity in congenitally versus late blind adults.
Brain and Language. 122(3), 162 - 170.
[PDF] (1.23 MB)
(2011).

Anatomical connectivity patterns predict face selectivity in the fusiform gyrus.
Nature Neuroscience. 15(2), 321 - 327.
[PDF] (1.07 MB)
(2011).

Differential selectivity for dynamic versus static information in face-selective cortical regions.
NeuroImage. 56(4), 2356 - 2363.
[PDF] (632.79 KB)
(2011).

Us and Them: Intergroup Failures of Empathy.
Current Directions in Psychological Science. 20(3), 149 - 153.
[PDF] (379.95 KB)
(2011).

Measuring the Development of Social Attention Using Free-Viewing.
Infancy. 17(4), 355 - 375.
[PDF] (487.66 KB)
(2011).

When ignorance is no excuse: Different roles for intent across moral domains.
Cognition. 120(2), 202 - 214.
[PDF] (794.54 KB)Stimuli
(2011).

Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108(7), 2688 - 2692.
[PDF] (257.86 KB)
(2011).

Associations and dissociations between default and self-reference networks in the human brain.
NeuroImage. 55(1), 225 - 232.
[PDF] (1.15 MB)
(2011).

Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108(11), 4429 - 4434.
[PDF] (400.4 KB)
(2011).

Neural evidence for “intuitive prosecution”: The use of mental state information for negative moral verdicts.
Social Neuroscience. 6(3), 302 - 315.
[PDF] (939.29 KB)
(2011).

fMRI item analysis in a theory of mind task.
NeuroImage. 55(2), 705 - 712.
[PDF] (495.53 KB)
(2011).

2010
The right temporo-parietal junction: a specific brain region for thinking about thoughts.
Handbook of Theory of Mind.
[PDF] (225.75 KB)
(2010).

Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 107(15), 6753 - 6758.
[PDF] (302.85 KB)
(2010).

Sensitive Period for a Multimodal Response in Human Visual Motion Area MT/MST.
Current Biology. 20(21), 1900 - 1906.
[PDF] (1.34 MB)Stimuli
(2010).

Attitudes towards the outgroup are predicted by activity in the precuneus in Arabs and Israelis.
NeuroImage. 52(4), 1704 - 1711.
[PDF] (720.52 KB)
(2010).

What gets the attention of the temporo-parietal junction? An fMRI investigation of attention and theory of mind.
Neuropsychologia. 48(9), 2658 - 2664.
[PDF] (626 KB)Stimuli
(2010).

It’s Not Just What You Do, but What’s on Your Mind: A Review of Kwame Anthony Appiah’s “Experiments in Ethics”.
Neuroethics. 31(3), 201 - 207.
[PDF] (384.47 KB)
(2010).

Investigating the Neural and Cognitive Basis of Moral Luck: It’s Not What You Do but What You Know.
Review of Philosophy and Psychology. 11(3), 333 - 349.
[PDF] (358.34 KB)
(2010).

Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 107(15), 6753 - 6758.
[PDF] (302.85 KB)
Young_2010_PNAS.pdf (302.85 KB)
(2010).


Live face-to-face interaction during fMRI: A new tool for social cognitive neuroscience.
NeuroImage. 50(4), 1639 - 1647.
[PDF] (1.06 MB)
(2010).

Just do it? Investigating the gap between prediction and action in toddlers’ causal inferences.
Cognition. 115(1), 104 - 117.
[PDF] (414.67 KB)
(2010).

2009
Theory of Mind (Neural Basis).
Encyclopedia of Consciousness.
[PDF] (103.86 KB)
(2009).

The happiness of the fish: Evidence for a common theory of one's own and others' actions.
Handbook of imagination and mental simulation. 257-265.
[PDF] (139.47 KB)
(2009).

Growing up blind does not change the neural bases of Theory of Mind.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106(27), 11312 - 11317.
[PDF] (534.32 KB)
(2009).

Action understanding as inverse planning.
Cognition. 113(3), 329 - 349.
[PDF] (718.57 KB)
(2009).

Growing up blind does not change the neural bases of Theory of Mind.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106(27), 11312 - 11317.
[PDF] (534.32 KB)
(2009).

The neural evidence for simulation is weaker than I think you think it is.
Philosophical Studies. 144(3), 447 - 456.
[PDF] (141.61 KB)
(2009).

Introduction to a Special Section of Developmental Social Cognitive Neuroscience.
Child Development. 80(4), 946 - 951.
[PDF] (69.42 KB)
(2009).

Brain Regions for Perceiving and Reasoning About Other People in School-Aged Children.
Child Development. 80(4), 1197 - 1209.
[PDF] (431.14 KB)Stimuli
(2009).

Distinct Regions of Right Temporo-Parietal Junction Are Selective for Theory of Mind and Exogenous Attention.
(Lauwereyns, J., Ed.).PLoS ONE. 4(3), e4869.
[PDF] (522.93 KB)
(2009).

Innocent intentions: A correlation between forgiveness for accidental harm and neural activity☆.
Neuropsychologia. 47(10), 2065 - 2072.
[PDF] (838.59 KB)Stimuli
(2009).

An fMRI Investigation of Spontaneous Mental State Inference for Moral Judgment.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 21(7), 1396 - 1405.
[PDF] (221.11 KB)
(2009).

2008
Concepts Are More than Percepts: The Case of Action Verbs.
Journal of Neuroscience. 28(44), 11347 - 11353.
[PDF] (202.54 KB)
(2008).

The influence of prior record on moral judgment.
Neuropsychologia. 46(12), 2949 - 2957.
[PDF] (1.48 MB)
(2008).

The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment.
NeuroImage. 40(4), 1912 - 1920.
[PDF] (363.71 KB)Stimuli
(2008).

2007
Goal Inference as Inverse Planning.
Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. 29(29), 779 - 784.
[PDF] (201.3 KB)
(2007).

Knowing who dunnit: Infants identify the causal agent in an unseen causal interaction..
Developmental Psychology. 43(1), 149 - 158.
[PDF] (600.56 KB)
(2007).

The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104(20), 8235 - 8240.
[PDF] (992.31 KB)
(2007).

2006
Divide and conquer: A defense of functional localizers.
NeuroImage. 30(4), 1088 - 1096.
[PDF] (296.13 KB)
(2006).

Bayesian models of human action understanding.
Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. 18, 99 - 106.
[PDF] (159.5 KB)
(2006).

Uniquely human social cognition.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 16(2), 235 - 239.
[PDF] (162.12 KB)
(2006).

Why and how to study Theory of Mind with fMRI.
Brain Research. 1079(1), 57 - 65.
[PDF] (177.01 KB)
(2006).

The perception of causality in infancy.
Acta Psychologica. 123(1-2), 144 - 165.
[PDF] (362.51 KB)
(2006).

It's the Thought That Counts.
Psychological Science. 17(8), 692 - 699.
[PDF] (673.88 KB)Stimuli
(2006).

My Body or Yours? The Effect of Visual Perspective on Cortical Body Representations.
Cerebral Cortex. 16(2), 178 - 182.
[PDF] (493.15 KB)
(2006).

Overlapping and non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individual subjects.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 115(3), 229 - 234.
[PDF] (377.94 KB)
(2006).

Reading minds versus following rules: Dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the brain.
Social Neuroscience. 1(3-4), 284 - 298.
[PDF] (427.88 KB)
(2006).

2005
Dissociation between emotion and personality judgments: Convergent evidence from functional neuroimaging.
NeuroImage. 28(4), 770 - 777.
[PDF] (1.4 MB)
(2005).

Against simulation: the argument from error.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9(4), 174 - 179.
[PDF] (237.67 KB)
(2005).

Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction.
Neuropsychologia. 43(10), 1391 - 1399.
[PDF] (275.42 KB)Stimuli
(2005).

Secret Agents: Inferences About Hidden Causes by 10- and 12-Month-Old Infants.
Psychological Science. 16(12), 995 - 1001.
[PDF] (548.7 KB)
(2005).

2004
Understanding Other Minds: Linking Developmental Psychology and Functional Neuroimaging.
Annual Review of Psychology. 55(1), 87 - 124.
[PDF] (347.58 KB)
(2004).

A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions.
Neuropsychologia. 42(11), 1435 - 1446.
[PDF] (361.65 KB)
(2004).

2003
People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”.
NeuroImage. 19(4), 1835 - 1842.
[PDF] (228.81 KB)Stimuli
(2003).
